|
13th International Conference
on Functional Grammar
|
Non-straightforward ascription |
Kees Hengeveld &
Evelien Keizer, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Amsterdam,
The Netherlands
In FDG (Hengeveld &
Mackenzie 2008) the referential or ascriptive status of a
linguistic unit is decided at the Interpersonal Level, since
it is the speaker who
refers to entities by using referring expressions, and it is
the speaker who ascribes properties to entities by applying
predicates to these referring expressions (Lyons 1977: 166,
171; Keizer 1992, 2008; Hengeveld 2004: 6). Reference
and ascription are captured by Referential (R) and Ascriptive
(T) Subacts, which are used in building
up a Communicated Content (C). Ascription is pervasive in the
model, since in most cases reference can only be established
through ascription. Thus, example (1) contains four
Ascriptive Subacts, one (TI) corresponding with the
lexeme that acts as the main predicate bought,
and three (TJ-TL) corresponding with the
lexemes that are used to build up Referential Subacts
corresponding to the two arguments the
man and a red car.
(1) The man bought a red
car.
Interpersonal
Level
|
CI :[
|
(TI ) |
(RI:
(TJ ) |
(RI)) |
(RJ :[
|
(TK) |
(TL)
]
|
(RI))](CI)) |
|
(fi: buy
(fi)) |
(xi: (fj:
man (fj)) |
(xi))A
|
(xj |
(fk: car
(fk)) (xj): |
(fl: red
(fl)) |
(xj))U
|
Representational
Level |
The
Ascriptive Subacts in (1) are cases of straightforward
ascription, in the sense that the various properties expressed
by the lexical elements are presented by the speaker without
modification. There are many cases, however, in which a
speaker does not simply ascribe selected properties, and there
is a range of linguistic strategies that can be interpreted as
encoding various types of non-straightforward ascription.
These include the following:
(i) dummy
strategies: when the speaker cannot find the appropriate
lexeme or for other reasons does not want to disclose the
information connected with that lexeme, dummy elements may act
as fillers of Ascriptive Subacts (e.g. thingummy)
(ii) approximation
strategies: when the speaker feels the lexeme chosen only
approximately serves his/her purposes, an approximative
element may be used (e.g. sort-of)
(iii) reportative
strategies: when the speaker wants to attribute the choice of
lexemes to others, he/she may use a reportative element (e.g. so-called).
Our talk consists of two
parts. In the first part, we present the strategies listed
here from a cross-linguistic perspective, showing the
relevance of the distinctions made, and arguing on the basis
of distributional arguments that these indeed belong to the
layer of the Ascriptive Subact at the Interpersonal Level in
FDG. In the second part, we show how the various strategies
are relevant to the grammar of English. A corpus-based
analysis reveals (a) that for each of the three functions
mentioned English has a large number of expressions available;
(b) that the form of these expressions varies not only
according to the type of ascription involved, but also
according to the denotation of the head; (c) that many of
these expressions also operate at other layers; and (d) that
it may not always be easy to distinguish between approximation
and mitigation.
|
|
|
References: |
-
Hengeveld,
K. 2004. The architecture of a Functional Discourse
Grammar. In: J. L. Mackenzie and M. Á. Gómez-González (eds),
A new architecture for Functional Grammar.Berlin:
Mouton de Gruyter, 1-21.
-
Hengeveld,
K. & Mackenzie, J.L. 2008. Functional Discourse Grammar: A typologically
based theory of language structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
-
Keizer,
M.E. 1992. Reference,
predication and (in)definiteness in Functional Grammar. A
functional approach to English copular sentences. PhD
dissertation, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.
-
Keizer,
M.E. 2008. Reference and Ascription in F(D)G: an inventory
of problem and some possible solutions. In:
D. García Velasco and J. Rijkhoff (eds) The Noun
Phrase in F(D)G. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 181-220.
-
Lyons,
J. 1977. Semantics, 2 volumes. Cambridge: CUP.
|
|
|